I have mentioned several times in recent weeks that I have been working on a paper dealing with adverse childhood experiences and adult mental illness - more specifically a rejection of the bio-bio-bio model of psychology that currently is dominating the landscape and offering support for more integrated biopsychosocial model (which used to be "the thing" a decade ago).
This is roughly the first 13 page of the text (with references at the end). It is a VERY rough draft and has not been edited more than a cursory once over to ensure it makes at least a little sense. When I do revise, I'm sure it will grow with more references and more explanation.
Feedback is welcome.
Adverse Childhood Experiences and Our Conceptions of Mental Illness
William Harryman, MSC, NCC, MS
The world of counseling and psychotherapy is rapidly approaching a potential future in which it no longer exists in its current form due to the rise of the biomedical model of mental illness and of psychopharmacology as the primary research and treatment avenue. Even the Federal government is backing the biomedical model. On 2 April 2013, President Barack Obama announced the Brain Research through Advancing Innovative Neurotechnologies (BRAIN) Initiative, with a requested $100 million dollars in funding for the 2014 fiscal year (Insel, Landis & Collins, 2013). In March of 2014, President Obama requested an increase in funding to $200 million for fiscal year 2015 (NIH, 2014). There is nothing remotely comparable in funding available for research into psychosocial intervention models. How we (counselors, social workers, psychotherapists) navigate the next decade or so could determine if there is a future for counseling and psychotherapy. In fact, we must do much more than navigate this difficult situation—we must advocate for our profession, for psychosocial causes and treatments for mental illness, and for a greater understanding of how adverse childhood experiences contribute to nearly all mental illness, from depression to anxiety disorders, from personality disorders to the various psychoses.
* * *
In 2005, Dr. Steven Sharfstein, then president of the American Psychiatric Association, wrote an article called, "Big Pharma and American Psychiatry: The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly." He was worried, and rightfully so, that psychiatry had been reduced to a pill-pushing profession. He warned that if psychiatrists were perceived to be "mere pill pushers and employees of the pharmaceutical industry, our credibility as a profession is compromised.” Sharfstein went so far as to urge his colleagues to "examine the fact that as a profession, we have allowed the bio-psychosocial model to become the bio-bio-bio model."
The end result of this unfortunate disregard for the incredibly well-documented impact of a dysfunctional early environment on mental health issues in adults (Edwards, et al, 2003; Anda, et al, 2006; Bemporad & Romano, 1993; Famularo et al., 1992; Silverman et al., 1996) is that the National Institutes for Mental Health (NIMH) earlier this year declared that only research oriented toward identifying organic, biological causes for mental illness would receive funding. NIMH director Thomas Insel has set out a very limited agenda for future funding, as outlined in his February 27, 2014 NIMH Director’s Blog.
Insel is mandating three serious changes to how the NIMH funds future research, but it is the first proposed change that is most relevant to those who favor a more integrated model for the etiology of mental illness:First, future trials will follow an experimental medicine approach in which interventions serve not only as potential treatments, but as probes to generate information about the mechanisms underlying a disorder. Trial proposals will need to identify a target or mediator; a positive result will require not only that an intervention ameliorated a symptom, but that it had a demonstrable effect on a target, such as a neural pathway implicated in the disorder or a key cognitive operation. (Insel, 2014)This approach seems short-sighted in that many new drugs are discovered to be effective in reducing some type of symptom, but the mechanism of action remains unclear for years or even decades. In the well-known Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) trial (Trivedi, Rush, Wisniewski, 2006), a range of medications that impact serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine showed no statistical differences in effect, suggesting that the commonly accepted notion of depression as a serotonin-based illness is not supported by the evidence. Considering this outcome, would Insel’s NIMH fund such a study under these new guidelines?
In the blog post referenced above, Insel goes on to say that there will be funding set aside for other approaches:Moreover, a subset of the funding announcements will support clinical trials that evaluate the effectiveness or increase the clinical impact of pharmacological, somatic, psychosocial, rehabilitative, and combination interventions. (Insel, 2014)It remains unclear how these projects might be funded if they are made to conform to the criteria presented above, although there is some acceptance of manualized treatment protocols such as cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and interpersonal therapy (IP). A psychosocial intervention is likely not going to offer a single target gene, neural circuit, or brain region as the mediator in a given mental illness. Despite this, in the STAR*D trial mentioned above, CBT outperformed all of the medications but took slightly longer to do so (Rush, Trivedi, Wisniewski, et al., 2006).Relationship of Early Adverse Experience on Adult Mental Health
We now have incredible piles of research, and decades of clinical evidence, that adverse childhood events are associated with mental health issues, including depression, anxiety, PTSD, personality disorders, and substance use disorders (Kessler et al., 2010; Read & Bentell, 2012). Even serious mental illnesses often thought to be genetic, such as schizophrenia and psychosis, have been profoundly linked to childhood trauma (Bebbington, 2009; Larkin & Read, 2008; Varese, et al., 2012). In fact, the genetic evidence for causation in schizophrenia is somewhat mixed, while there is substantial support for environmental causes. The National Institutes of Health (NIH) states that there are roughly 2.4 million Americans with schizophrenia, but then admits a few sentences later that genetic mutations can only explain a few thousand of these cases. Here are the comments by Dr. Francis Crick (2014):Over the last five years, multiple research projects known as genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have identified dozens of common variations in the human genome associated with increased risk of schizophrenia [2]. However, the individual effects of these variants are weak, and it’s often not been clear which genes were actually affected by the variations. Now, advances in DNA sequencing technology have made it possible to move beyond these association studies to study the actual DNA sequence of the protein-coding region of the entire genome for thousands of individuals with schizophrenia. Reports just published have revealed a complex constellation of rare mutations that point to specific genes—at least in certain cases.These are not convincing comments on the state of genetic research. A very recent article on environmental causes for schizophrenia cites new GWAS research that offers a much lower causative factor for genes as opposed to environment:In a new GWAS study, Ripkeetal (2013) estimated that in schizophrenia about 8.300 [single nucleotide polymorphisms] SNPs contribute to a common risk of 32%, suggesting that environmental factors interacting with the genetic background contribute to the pathophysiology (Manolioetal, 2009). In schizophrenia, environmental factors are proposed to play a role up to 60% (Benrosetal, 2011). (Schmitt, Malchow, Hasan, and Falkai, 2014)Based on this research, environmental factors are twice as likely to cause schizophrenia as are genetic factors and yet the research community seems to buy into the genetic causation model.
According to the DSM-IV-TR, there are five “characteristic symptoms” of schizophrenia: hallucinations, delusions, thought disorder (disorganized thinking), grossly disorganized or catatonic behavior, and negative symptoms (APA 2000). One inpatient study (Read and Argyle 1999) found one or more of these symptoms in 75% of those subjects who had suffered childhood physical abuse (CPA), in 76% of those who had suffered childhood sexual abuse (CSA), and in 100% of those subjected to incest. In a study of outpatient schizophrenics, 35% had experienced emotional abuse, 42% had experienced physical neglect, and 73% had experienced emotional neglect (Holowka et al, 2003). The same study found that among adult outpatients diagnosed with schizophrenia, 85% had experienced some form of childhood abuse or neglect, 73% emotional neglect, and 50% sexual abuse.
A review of 40 studies in which 50% of the subjects had been diagnosed psychotic, both inpatient and outpatient, found that half of the women had experienced childhood sexual abuse, and a majority of women (69%) and men (60%) experienced either childhood sexual or physical abuse (Read et al, 2004). Read, van Os, Morrison, and Ross (2005) report that patients subjected to CSA or CPA have “earlier first admissions, longer and more frequent hospitalizations, spend longer in seclusion, receive more medication, are more likely to self-mutilate and to try to kill themselves, and have higher global symptom severity” (Beck & van der Kolk, 1987; Briere et al, 1997; Garno et al, 2005; Goff et al, 1991; Lange et al, 1995; Lipschitz et al, 1996; Mullen et al, 1993; Read, 1998; Read et al, 2001; Rose, Peabody, & Stratigeas, 1991; Sansonnet-Hayden et al, 1987). In another study, childhood abuse was a better predictor of suicidality in adult out-patients than was a current depression diagnosis (Read et al, 2001).
Childhood abuse and neglect are possibly more related to psychoses such as schizophrenia than they are to anxiety or mood disorders.Many studies, however, provide further evidence that child abuse and neglect may be even more strongly related to diagnoses of schizophrenia than to diagnoses indicating less severe disturbance. (Read, Rudegeair & Farrelly, 2006)This is an important statement. If this is true, there must be an effort to prevent abuse and neglect at the same level in which there is an effort to cure HIV or to cure cancer. A society should be judged by how well it takes care of its weak and vulnerable, and there are no members of this society as vulnerable as are children. We are failing them—and in the most extreme outcomes, they respond to a world that is unfathomable and intolerable with psychosis.
* * *
In the 1950s, schizophrenia was often conceptualized as a disease resulting from poor parenting skills on the part of the mother and in her use of the “double bind” in verbal and non-verbal interactions with the child. Gregory Bateson (1956) was the primary architect of this theory, and it still echoes in the folk psychology of schizophrenia. Here is a definition of the “double bind” as given in Bateson’s seminal paper on the subject:We hypothesize that there will be a breakdown in any individual's ability to discriminate between Logical Types whenever a double bind situation occurs. The general characteristics of this situation are the following:In point two Bateson presents the fundamental conflict of the double bind: the caretaker (rather than always the mother) feels hostility or anxiety with the child, but instead of processing those feelings they get pushed down and she tries to pretend she feels nothing but love for the child. For the child, this creates a situation of having to choose between the words or the actions (the somatic experience) of the caretaker, forcing them into having to deny or disregard one or more aspects of their own experience. Furthermore, the child is forbidden either implicitly or explicitly from objecting to the bind in which she is place.
1. When the individual is involved in an intense relationship; that is, a relationship in which he feels it is vitally important that he discriminate accurately what sort of message is being communicated so that he may respond appropriately.
2. And, the individual is caught in a situation in which the other person in the relationship is expressing two orders of message and one of these denies the other.
3. And, the individual is unable to comment on the messages being expressed to correct his discrimination of what order of message to respond to, i.e., he cannot make a meta-communicative statement. (Bateson et al., 1956)
This act of denial can stem from something as simple as the mother saying, “Mommy loves you,” while simultaneously being physically rigid or emotionally distant. To that end, Bateson and his team make the following assertion:[W]e hypothesize that the mother of a schizophrenic will be simultaneously expressing at least two orders of message. These orders of message can be roughly characterized as (a) hostile or withdrawing behavior which is aroused whenever the child approaches her, and (b) simulated loving or approaching behavior which is aroused when the child responds to her hostile and withdrawing behavior, as a way of denying that she is withdrawing. (Bateson et al., 1956)For the child in this situation, there is no good option. He must reject his own inner sense of the mother’s emotional state and accept her false loving behaviors. Thus he is in a double bind.The only way the child can really escape from the situation is to comment on the contradictory position his mother has put him in. However, if he did so, the mother would take this as an accusation that she is unloving and both punish him and insist that his perception of the situation is distorted. (Bateson et al, 1956)The obvious fallacy in this model is that parents who place their children in a double bind will have children who become schizophrenic. Clearly, there is a lot of double bind communication in families that never results in psychosis or schizophrenia. It is more likely, however, that double bind communication patterns are one of the environmental factors leading to schizophrenia and, it seems logical to assume, other forms of mental illness (as already outlined above).
Bugenthal et al. (1971) found that in families with “disturbed members” (children who were referred by schools because of disruptive behavior and chronic emotional problems), a greater number of mothers engaged in conflicting verbal-nonverbal communication than did mothers in families without “disturbed members” during a five-minute period families were observed discussing family related issues (cited in Koopmans, 1997). This finding seems to add some more foundation to the power of double bind and conflicting communications to have negative impacts on some children, likely those with genetic vulnerability and/or those who have experienced other forms of abuse or neglect.
* * *
While one might be tempted to assume that harsh or highly demonstrative parental communication (yelling, screaming, and so on) would be at the root of mental illness in their offspring, the opposite seems to be the case. Considerable research (Gross & Levenson, 1997; Jenkins, Karno, de la Selva, & Santana, 1986; Left & Vaughn, 1985) has shown that it is low emotional expression that is most common in families with schizophrenic children. This defies the folk understanding of the etiology of mental illness, yet it also explains the observed “confusion of message and meta-message” in the communication of schizophrenics (Bateson, et al., 1962). In a family with low emotional expression, the double bind might be, “We love you” (the first order message), “But we don’t show emotions in this family” (the second order message). On top of that contradiction, the child is also forbidden to be aware of or to mention the contradiction.
In more obviously abusive families, especially sexual abuse, there is profound connection between double binds and dissociation. David Spiegel (1986) suggests that the double bind is a primary factor is dissociative experience in individuals who are disposed to such experience (which is equivalent to being easily hypnotized).There is a triggering traumatic event—the first episode of sexual abuse—that is repeated over time in a family environment that all too often fails to restoratively provision the victim. Rather, the family double binds the child by insisting that she simultaneously become a demure “good girl” and a sultry sexual partner to the perpetrator and further enjoins her not to know about or point out the contradictory demands. (Davies & Frawley, 1994)In order to cope with both the abuse and the double bind, the child dissociates. Over time the child will resort to dissociation, because it works, whenever reality becomes too averse. To extend this out into the adult life of this child, if the abuse continues and the double binds continue, there is a much higher risk of psychosis, as the statistics presented about illustrate.
The renowned psychoanalyst, and co-founder (with Robert Stolorow and Donna Orange) of the intersubjective systems theory of relational psychoanalytic therapy (Atwood & Stolorow, 1984; Stolorow et al., 1987; Stolorow & Atwood, 1992; Orange, 1995; Orange et al., 1997), George Atwood, posits that for some who fall into psychosis, the cause is a profound enmeshment with the caregiver in childhood (Atwood, 2011). In line with Atwood’s thinking, but using different terminology, Munich and Munich (2009) outline what they term “overparenting”:In psychological terms, overparenting includes an excessive involvement with and concern about the child’s mental state and adaptive capacity that leads to a relative absence of space for the development of structuralized self and object relations. This also involves difficulty with separation, especially evident as the child leaves home.According to these authors, we are seeing the results of this “helicopter parenting” in college-age young adults.The sequelae of this level of parental involvement can be seen in a dramatic increase in demand for college counseling services for students with severe adjustment disorders, presentations of low self-esteem, and ongoing difficulties in feeling autonomous, as well as disorders of the self, represented not as much by cognitive or instrumental deficits, but more with feelings of emptiness and confusion about direction. (Munich and Munich, 2009)According to Atwood, this type of overparenting or enmeshment can lead to a sense of annihilation for the child, and annihilation in this context is the “abyss of madness.”An accommodation has taken place at a very young age in which the agenda of the caregiver–it can be the mother, the father, or both–becomes the supreme principle defining the child’s developing sense of personal identity. The experience of the child as an independent person in his or her own right is nullified, so that they child the parents wish for can be brought into being. Very often there are no outward signs of anything amiss, as family life unfolds in a seeming harmony. Somewhere along the way, however, the false self begins to crumble, and a sense of the degree to which the child has been absent from life arises. (Atwood, 2011, p. 60)For the child, there is the sense of emptiness mentioned above by Munich and Munich, of never having been a unique self. There is the very real sense of having been controlled and manipulated by outside forces. This sense of emptiness and external control may take on concrete reality for the child, who is now at least a teen or young adult, as a delusion that begins to replace reality, for example in the image of an influencing machine (Tausk, 1933; Orange et al., 1997). “What looks like a breakdown into psychosis and delusion thus may represent an attempted breakthrough, but the inchoate ‘I’ does require an understanding and responsive ‘Thou’ in order to have a chance to consolidate itself” (Atwood, 2011, p. 61).
* * *
Returning to the topic of seeking biological explanations and treatments for psychosis and schizophrenia, the synthesis of the first antipsychotic medication, Thorazine (chlorpromazine hydrochloride), in 1951 and its subsequent use as the primary treatment for schizophrenia in the late 1950s and early 1960s, resulting in a shift for the whole model of treating mental illness from psychosocial to biological. At the time, there was a power struggle between the old guard of psychoanalysis on one side, and the rise of biological psychiatry on the other (Rissmiller & Rissmiller, 2006). “Chlorpromazine was instrumental in the development of neuropsychopharmacology, a new discipline dedicated to the study of mental pathology with the employment of centrally acting drugs” (Ban, 2007). Thorazine led to tricyclic antidepressants in the 1960s and, eventually, to the creation of selective-serotonin-reuptake-inhibitors (SSRI) antidepressants, the first one being fluoxetine (Prozac), discovered in 1974 (Wong, et al.) and approved by the Food and Drug Administration in 1987.
With the rise of psychopharmacology to manage mental illness, the “mental asylum” became a thing of the past. In 1955, the mentally ill population in public psychiatric hospitals peaked at 560,000 (Pan, 2013). By 2004, the number of seriously mentally ill people in jails and prisons outnumbers those in hospitals by three-to-one—320,000 people in prison (16% of all inmates) and about 100,000 in public and private hospitals (Pan, 2013). According to the Bureau of Justice Statistics (2006), the numbers are much higher—by midyear of 2005 more than half of all prison and jail inmates had a mental health problem,including 705,600 inmates in State prisons, 78,800 in Federal prisons, and 479,900 in local jails. These estimates represented 56% of State prisoners, 45% of Federal prisoners, and 64% of jail inmates. (BJS, 2006)We have literally recreated the “bedlam” approach to housing the mentally ill. More alarming, perhaps, is that only 1 in 3 State prisoners and 1 in 6 jail inmates with mental health issues are getting any form of treatment after admission (BJS, 2006).
As the number of people in psychiatric hospitals declined over the past four decades, so did the number of psychiatrists treating those people with anything other than medications. According to a 2011 New York Times article (Harris):A 2005 government survey found that just 11 percent of psychiatrists provided talk therapy to all patients, a share that had been falling for years and has most likely fallen more since. Psychiatric hospitals that once offered patients months of talk therapy now discharge them within days with only pills.We are left, then, with the NIMH only offering funding for research that adheres to a biological model of mental illness, with more mentally ill adults in prisons than in psychiatric hospitals (where few of them are receiving treatment), and with only 11% of psychiatrists still offering therapy to all of their clients. Clearly, the biological model has won the battle.
So, then, what are we to do with all of the clinical and research evidence of how intensely early adverse relational experiences impact adult mental health status?
References
Bagaimana Menarikkan Article Pada Hari Ini . BLUE.Jangan Lupa Datang Lagi Untuk Membaca Article Yang lebih Menarik Pada Masa Akan Datang/Insel, T., Story C. Landis, S.C., & Collins, F.S. (2013, May 10). The NIH BRAIN Initiative. Science; 340(6133): 687-688. DOI: 10.1126/science.1239276National Institutes of Health. (2014, Mar 11). President Obama’s Proposal to Double Federal Funding for the BRAIN Initiative. NIH BRAIN Initiative blog; retrieved from http://brainfeedback.nih.gov/president-obamas-proposal-to-double-federal-funding-for-the-brain-initiative/ on March 26, 2014.Sharfstein, S.S. (2005, Aug 19). Big Pharma and American Psychiatry: The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly. Psychiatric News of the American Psychiatric Society. Retrieved March 18, 2014, from http://psychnews.psychiatryonline.org/newsArticle.aspx?articleid=109213Anda, R.F., Felitti, V.J., Bremner, J.D., Walker, J.D., Whitfield, C., Perry, B.D., Dube, S.R., Giles, W.H. (2006, Apr). The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood: A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neuroscience; 256(3):174-86.Bemporad, J.R., Romano, S. (1993). Childhood experience and adult depression: a review of studies. Am J Psychoanal 53:301-315Edwards, V.J., Holden, G.W., Felitti, V.J., and Anda, R.F. (2003). Relationship Between Multiple Forms of Childhood Maltreatment and Adult Mental Health in Community Respondents: Results From the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Am J Psychiatry; 160:1453-1460. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1453Famularo, R., Kinscherff, R., Fenton, T. (1992). Psychiatric diagnosis of maltreated children: preliminary findings. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry; 31:863-867Silverman, A.B., Reinherz, H.Z., Giaconia, R.M. (1996), The long term sequelae of child and adolescent abuse: a longitudinal community study. Child Abuse Negl; 20:709-723Insel, T. (2014, Feb 27). A New Approach to Clinical Trials. NIMH Director’s Blog, NIMH. Retrieved March 18, 2014 from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/director/2014/a-new-approach-to-clinical-trials.shtmlTrivedi MH, Rush AJ, Wisniewski SR, et al. (2006). Evaluation of outcomes with citalopram for depression using measurement-based care in STAR*D: implications for clinical practice. Am J Psychiatry; 163(1):28–40.Rush AJ, Trivedi, MH, Wisniewski SR, et al. (2006). Acute and Longer-Term Outcomes in Depressed Outpatients Requiring One or Several Treatment Steps: A STAR*D Report. Am J Psychiatry; 163(11):1905–1917.Kessler R, McLaughlin K, Green J et al. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Br. J. Psychiatry 197, 378–385.Read J, Bentall R. (2012). Negative childhood experiences and mental health: theoretical, clinical and primary prevention implications. Br. J. Psychiatry 200, 89–91.Bebbington, P. (2009). Childhood sexual abuse and psychosis: Aetiology and mechanism. Epidemiological and Psychiatric Sciences; 18, 284–293.Larkin W, Read J. (2008). Childhood trauma and psychosis: Evidence, pathways, and implications. Journal of Postgraduate Medicine; 54, 284–290.Varese P, Smeets F, Drukker M et al. (2012). Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin; 38, 661–671.Crick, F. (2014, Jan 28). Exploring the Complex Genetics of Schizophrenia. NIH Director’s Blog, NIH. Retrieved March 18, 2014 from http://directorsblog.nih.gov/2014/01/28/exploring-the-complex-genetics-of-schizophrenia/Read, J. (2004). A history of madness. In J. Read, L. Mosher, and R. Bentall (eds) Models of Madness (pp. 9-20). Hove: Brunner-Routledge.Schmitt, A., Malchow, B., Hasan, A., and Falkai, P. (2014, Feb). The impact of environmental factors in severe psychiatric disorders. Frontiers in Neuroscience; 8(19). doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00019American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: APA.Read, J., and Argyle, N. (1999). Hallucinations, delusions and thought disorders among adult psychiatric inpatients with a history of child abuse. Psychiatric Services, 50, 1467–1472.Holowka DW, King S, Saheb D, Pukall M, Brunet A. (2003, Mar 1). Childhood abuse and dissociative symptoms in adult schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research; 60(1): 87-90.Read, J., van Os, J., Morrison, A.P., and Ross, C.A. (2005). Childhood trauma, psychosis and schizophrenia: a literature review with theoretical and clinical implications. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica; 112: 330–350.Mullen, P., Martin, J., Anderson, J., Romans, S., and Herbison, G. (1993). Childhood sexual abuse and mental health in adult life. Br J Psychiatry; 163:721–732.Lange A, Kooiman K, Huberts L, Van Oostendorp E. (1995). Childhood unwanted sexual events and degree of psychopathology of psychiatric patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand; 92:441–446.Beck J, Van Der Kolk B. (1987). Reports of childhood incest and current behavior of chronically hospitalized psychotic women. Am J Psychiatry; 144:1474–1476.Briere J, Woo R, Mcrae B, Foltz J, Sitzman R. (1997). Lifetime victimization history, demographics, and clinical status in Childhood trauma and psychosis female psychiatric emergency room patients. J Nerv Ment Dis; 85:95–101.Garno J, Goldberg J, Ramirez P et al. (2005). Impact of childhood abuse on the clinical course of bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry; 186:121–125.Goff D, Brotman A, Kindlon D, Waites M, Amico E. (1991). Self-reports of child abuse in chronically psychotic patients. Psychiatry Res; 37:73–80.Lipschitz D, Kaplan M, Sorkenn J, Faedda G, Chorney P, Asnis G. (1996). Prevalence and characteristics of physical and sexual abuse among psychiatric outpatients. Psychiatr Serv; 47:189–191.Read J. (1998). Child abuse and severity of disturbance among adult psychiatric inpatients. Child Abuse Negl; 22:359–368.Read J, Agar K, Barker-Collo S, Davies E, Moskowitz A. (2001). Assessing suicidality in adults: integrating childhood trauma as a major risk factor. Prof Psychol Res Pr; 32:367–372.Rose S, Peabody C, Stratigeas B. (1991). Undetected abuse among intensive case management clients. Hosp Community Psychiatry; 42:499–503.Sansonnet-Hayden H, Haley G, Marriage K, Fine S. (1987). Sexual abuse and psychopathology in hospitalized adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry; 26:753–757.Read J, Agar K, Barker-Collo S, Davies E, Moskowitz A. (2001). Assessing suicidality in adults: integrating childhood trauma as a major risk factor. Prof Psychol Res Pr; 32:367–372.Read, J. Rudegeair, T., & Farrelly, S, (2006). Relationship between child abuse and psychosis: Public opinion, evidence, pathways and implications. In Trauma & psychosis: New directions for theory and therapy; Warren Larkin & Anthony P. Morrison (eds). NY: Routledge.Bateson, G., Jackson, D.D., Haley, J., and Weakland, J. (1956). Toward a Theory of Schizophrenia. Behavioral Science; 1(4): 251-254.Bugenthal, D.E., Love, L.R., Kaswan, J.W., & April, C. (1971). Verbal-nonverbal conflict in parental messages to normal and disturbed children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology; 77, 6-10.Koopmans, M. (1997). Schizophrenia and the Family: Double Bind Theory Revisited. Dynamical Psychology; Retrieved March 24, 2014, from http://www.goertzel.org/dynapsyc/1997/Koopmans.htmlJenkins, J.H., Kamo, M., de la Selva, S., & Santana, E. (1986). Expressed emotion in cross-cultural context: Familial responses to schizophrenia among Mexican Americans. In M. J. Goldstein (Ed.), Treatment of schizophrenia: Family assessment and intervention (pp. 35--49). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Left, J.P., & Vaughn, C. (1985). Expressed emotion in families: Its significance for mental illness. New York: Guilford.Gross, J.J., & Levenson, R.W. (1997). Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 95-103.Bateson, G., Jackson, D.D., Haley, J., & Weakland, J.H. (1963). A note on the double bind—1962. Family Process, 2(1), 154-161.Ban, T.A. (2007, Aug). Fifty years chlorpromazine: A historical perspective. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat; 3(4): 495–500.Spiegel, D. (1986). Dissociation, double binds, and posttraumatic stress disorder in multiple personality disorder. In B.G. Braun (ed.), Treatment of multiple personality disorder (61-78). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press.Davies, J.M. & Frawley, M.G. (1994). Treating the adult survivor of childhood sexual abuse: A psychoanalytic perspective. New York: BasicBooks/Harper Collins Publishers.Atwood, G., & Stolorow, R. (1984). Structures of Subjectivity: Explorations in Psychoanalytic Phenomenology. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.Stolorow, R., Brandchaft, B., Atwood, G. (1987). Psychoanalytic Treatment: An Intersubjective Approach. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.Stolorow, R., & Atwood, G. (1992). Contexts of Being: the Intersubjective Foundations of Psychological Life. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.Orange, D. (1995). Emotional Understanding: Studies in Psychoanalytic Epistemology. NY: Guilford.Orange, D., Atwood, G., Stolorow, R. (1997). Working Intersubjectively: Contextualism in Psychoanalytic Practice. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.Atwood, G. (2011). The Abyss of Madness. New York: Routledge.Munich, R.L, and Munich, M.A. (2009, Apr). Overparenting and the Narcissistic Pursuit of Attachment. Psychiatric Annals; 39(4): 227-235.Tausk V (1933). On the origin of the influencing machine in schizophrenia. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 2: 519-556.Rissmiller, D.J. & Rissmiller, J.H. (2006, Jun). Evolution of the Antipsychiatry Movement into Mental Health Consumerism. Psychiatric Services; 57(6): 863-866.Ban, T.A. (2007). Fifty years chlorpromazine: a historical perspective. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment; 3(4): 495–500.Wong, D.T., Horng, J.S., Bymaster, F.P., Hauser, K.L., Molloy, B.B. (1974). A selective inhibitor of serotonin uptake: Lilly 110140, 3-(p-Trifluoromethylphenoxy)-n-methyl-3-phenylpropylamine. Life Sciences; 15(3): 471–9. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(74)90345-2.Pan, D. (2013, Apr 29). Timeline: Deinstitutionalization and Its Consequences. Mother Jones; Retrieved from http://www.motherjones.com/politics/2013/04/timeline-mental-health-america on March, 25, 2014.Harris, G. (2001, Mar 5). Talk Doesn’t Pay, So Psychiatry Turns Instead to Drug Therapy. New York Times; page A1 of the New York edition. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/03/06/health/policy/06doctors.html on March 25, 2014.
Posting Komentar